设为首页 | 加入收藏
网站首页 本刊简介 编委会 投稿指南 过刊浏览 广告合作 网上订购 下载专区 联系我们  
意外创伤者创伤后成长认知加工偏向实验研究
作者:董超群1  巩树梅2  邓光辉3  辛松建4  刘晓虹2 
单位:1. 温州医科大学护理学院  温州 325035 
2.
 第二军医大学护理学院  上海 200433 
3.
 第二军医大学  心理与精神卫生学系  上海 200433 
4.
 温州医科大学附属第二医院康复科  温州 325027 
关键词:意外创伤者 创伤后成长 注意偏向 点探测任务 
分类号:R395
出版年,卷(期):页码:2014,22(4):601-604
摘要:

目的:考察不同创伤后成长(PTG)水平意外创伤者的注意偏向特点。方法:采用简体中文版创伤后成长问卷筛选出15名高分组被试和15名低分组被试,以积极、消极和中性表情图片为实验材料进行2(表情对性质:正性、负性)×2(探测点与表情图片-致性:-致、不-致)×2(高PTG水平、低PTG水平)混合设计的点探测实验。结果:低PTG水平的伤者对负性情绪表情图片存在注意偏向和注意脱离困难,注意偏向值(D)和脱离指数(DI)均显著大于0(P<0.05);高PTG水平的伤者对正性和负性情绪刺激均不存在注意偏向;在探测点与负性情绪表情图片位置不-致时,高PTG水平伤者的反应时要显著少于低PTG水平伤者,P<0.05。结论:PTG水平不同的伤者对情绪刺激的注意偏差存在不同,两者具有不同的内隐认知加工方式,可据此指导意外创伤者心理干预的开展。

Objective:To explore the characteristics of attention bias in accidentally injured patients with different levels of Posttraumatic growth(PTG).Methods: Total score of PTG was adopted to screen out 15 high-PTG group and lowPTG group respectively among accidentally injured patients.Dot probe task was used with positive, negative and neutral facial expression pictures as experimental materials.An experimental design of 2(facial expression: positive and negative)×2(consistency of probe point and facial expression: consistent and inconsistent)×2(PTG level: high and low) was employed.Results: Patients with low PTG level had attention bias toward the negative emotional stimuli, and difficulty in distraction from the negative emotional pictures.The value of D and DI were both significantly greater than 0(P0.05).Patients with high PTG level did not demonstrate significant attention bias toward positive or negative emotional stimuli.The responding time of patients with high PTG level was significantly shorter than that in patients with low PTG level in the incongruent task(P0.05).Conclusion: There are different characteristics of implicit cognitive processing in patients with different level of PTG, suggesting the necessity of psychological intervention on the accidentally injured patients.

基金项目:
国家自然科学基金(71371180)
作者简介:
参考文献:

[1] Gabert-Quillen CA, Fallon W, Delahanty DL. PTSD after traumatic injury: an investigation of the impact of injury severity and peritraumatic moderators. Journal of Health Psychology, 2011, 16(4): 678-687.
[2] Bryant RA, Harvey AG, Guthrie RM, et al. A prospective study of psychophysiological arousal, acute stress disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2000, 109(2):341-344.
[3] 张丽,郑会蓉,李卫晖,等慢性创伤后应激障碍患者的脑功能磁共振研究中国临床心理学杂志,2011,19(1):4-6.
[4] Liberzon I, Martis B. Neuroimaging studies of emotional responses in PTSD. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 2006, 1071(1): 87-109.
[5] Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry, 2004, 15(1): 1-18.
[6] Zoellner T, Rabe S, Karl A, et al. Posttraumatic growth in accident survivors: openness and optimism as predictors of its constructive or illusory sides. Journal of clinical Psychology, 2008, 64(3): 245-263.
[7] Weiss T, Berger R. Posttraumatic growth and culturally com-petent practice: Lessons learned from around the globe.Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 2010. 1-14.
[8] Beck JG, Freeman JB, Shipherd JC, et al. Specificity of st-roop interference in patients with pain and PTSD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2001, 110(4): 536-543.
[9] Bryant RA, Harvey AG. Attentional bias in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 1997, 10(4): 635-644.
[10] Chan MW, Ho SM, Tedeschi RG, et al. The valence of atten-tional bias and cancer-related rumination in posttraumatic stress and posttraumatic growth among women with breast cancer. Psychooncology, 2011, 20(5): 544-552.
[11] Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Mangun GR. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nature neuroscience, 2000, 3(3): 284-291.
[12] 苟娜PTSD初中生注意偏向的实验研究硕士学位论文重庆:西南大学,2009.
[13] 汪际,陈瑶,王艳波,等创伤后成长评定量表的修订及信效度分析护理学杂志,2011,26(14):26-28.
[14] Mogg K, Garner M, Bradley BP. Anxiety and orienting of gaze to angry and fearful faces. Biological Psychology, 2007,76(3): 163-169.
[15] 罗亚莉,张大均高特质愤怒者对负性情绪刺激注意偏向的潜在机制中国临床心理学杂志,2013,21(2):180-184.
[16] 史娟,李朝旭,张永慧,等癌症患者对情绪信息的注意偏向特点中国临床心理学杂志,2014,22(2):223-227.
[17] Salemink E, van den Hout MA, Kindt M. Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2007, 45(3): 607-615.
[18] 米熊兆,钟明天,蚁金瑶,等临床抑郁症患者的注意偏倚特征中国临床心理学杂志,2008,16(3):234-236.
[19] 任丽,刘爱书完美主义者对不完美图片的注意偏向中国临床心理学杂志,2013,21(4):541-544.
[20] Fox E, Ridgewell A, Ashwin C. Looking on the bright side: Biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2009, 276(1663): 1747-1751.
[21] Janoff-Bulman R. Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of trauma. Free Press, 1992. 115-141.
[22] Janoff-Bulman R. Posttraumatic growth: Three explanatory models. Psychological Inquiry, 2004, 15(1): 20-34.
[23] 聂玉风,陈国娜不同情绪状态对大学生注意偏向影响的实验研究三门峡职业技术学院学报,2012,11(4):51-54.

服务与反馈:
文章下载】【加入收藏
您是第访问者

《中国临床心理学杂志》编辑部
地址:湖南省长沙市中南大学湘雅二医院内, 410011
电 话:0731-85292472    电子邮件:cjcp_china@163.com
本系统由北京博渊星辰网络科技有限公司设计开发 技术支持电话:010-63361626